Cognitive Psychology: History Background of cognitive psychology
Cognitive Psychology: History
Since the beginning of experimental psychology in
the nineteenth century, there had been interest in the
study of higher mental processes. But something
discontinuous happened in the late 1950s, something
so dramatic that it is now referred to as the ‘cognitive
revolution,’ and the view of mental processes that it
spawned is called ‘cognitive psychology.’ What happened
was that American psychologists rejected behaviorism
and adopted a model of mind based on the
computer. The brief history that follows (adapted in
part from Hilgard (1987) and Kessel and Bevan (1985))
chronicles mainstream cognitive psychology from the
onset of the cognitive revolution to the beginning of
the twenty-first century.
1. Beginnings:
From roughly the 1920s through the 1950s, American
psychology was dominated by behaviorism. Behaviorism was concerned primarily with the learning of
associations, particularly in nonhuman species, and it
constrained theorizing to stimulus–response notions.
The overthrow of behaviorism came not so much from
ideas within psychology as from three research approaches
external to the field.
A) Communications Research and the Information
Processing Approach:
During World War II, new concepts and theories were
developed about signal processing and communication,
and these ideas had a profound impact on
psychologists active during the war years. One important
work was Shannon’s 1948 paper about Information
Theory. It proposed that information was
communicated by sending a signal through a sequence
of stages or transformations. This suggested that
human perception and memory might be conceptualized
in a similar way: sensory information enters
the receptors, then is fed into perceptual analyzers,
whose outputs in turn are input to memory systems.
This was the start of the ‘information processing’
approach—the idea that cognition could be understood
as a flow of information within the organism,
an idea that continues to dominate cognitive psychology.
Perhaps the first major theoretical effort in information
processing psychology was Donald Broadbent’s
Perception and Communication (Broadbent
1958). According to Broadbent’s model, information
output from the perceptual system encountered a
filter, which passed only information to which people
were attending. Although this notion of an all-or-none
filter would prove too strong (Treisman 1960).
B) The Computer Modeling Approach:
Technical developments during World War II also led
to the development of digital computers. Questions
soon arose about the comparability of computer and
human intelligence (Turing 1950). By 1957, Alan
Newell, J. C. Shaw, and Herb Simon had designed a
computer program that could solve difficult logic
problems, a domain previously thought to be the
unique province of humans. Newell and Simon soon
followed with programs that displayed general
problem-solving skills much like those of humans, and
argued that these programs offered detailed models of
human problem solving (a classic summary is contained
in Newell and Simon (1972)). This work would
also help establish the field of artificial intelligence.
Early on, cross-talk developed between the computer
modeling and information-processing approaches,
which crystallized in the 1960 book Plans
and the Structure of Behavior (Miller et al. 1960). The
book showed that information-processing psychology
could use the theoretical language of computer modeling
even if it did not actually lead to computer
programs.
C) The Generative Linguistics Approach:
A third external influence that lead to the rise of
modern cognitive psychology was the development of
generative grammar in linguistics by Noam Chomsky.
Two of Chomsky’s publications in the late 1950s had
a profound effect on the nascent cognitive psychology.
The first was his 1957 book Syntactic Structures
(Chomsky 1957). It focused on the mental structures
needed to represent the kind of linguistic knowledge
that any competent speaker of a language must have.
Chomsky argued that associations per se, and even
phrase structure grammars, could not fully represent
our knowledge of syntax (how words are organized
into phrases and sentences). What had to be added
was a component capable of transforming one syntactic
structure into another. These proposals about transnational grammar would change the intellectual
landscape of linguistics, and usher in a new
psycho linguistics.
Chomsky’s second publication (1959) was a review
of Verbal Behavior, a book about language learning by
the then most respected behaviorist alive, B. F. Skinner
(Skinner 1957). Chomsky’s review is arguably one of
the most significant documents in the history of
cognitive psychology. It aimed not merely to devastate
Skinner’s proposals about language, but to undermine
behaviorism as a serious scientific approach to psychology.
To some extent, it succeeded on both counts.
D) An Approach Intrinsic to Psychology:
At least one source of modern cognitive psychology
came from within the field. This approach had its roots
in Gestalt psychology, and maintained its focus on the
higher mental processes. A signal event in this tradition
was the 1956 book A Study of Thinking, by
Bruner, Goodnow, and Austin (Bruner et al. 1956).
The work investigated how people learn new concepts
and categories, and it emphasized strategies of learning
rather than just associative relations. The proposals fit
perfectly with the information-processing approach—
indeed, they were information processing proposals—
and offered still another reason to break from behaviorism.
2. The Growth of Cognitive Psychology:
The 1960s brought progress in many of the above mentioned
topic areas, some of which are highlighted
below:
A) Pattern Recognition:
One of the first areas to benefit from the cognitive
revolution was pattern recognition, the study of how
people perceive and recognize objects. The cognitive
approach provided a general two-stage view of object
recognition: (a) describing the input object in terms of relatively primitive features (e.g., ‘it has two diagonal
lines and one horizontal line connecting them’); and
(b) matching this object description to stored object
descriptions in visual memory, and selecting the best
match as the identity of the input object (‘this
description best matches the letter A’). This two Stages view was not entirely new to psychology, but expressing
it in information-processing terms allowed
one to connect empirical studies of object perception
to computer models of the process. The psychologist
Ulrich Neisser (1964) used a computer model of
pattern recognition (Selfridge 1959) to direct his
empirical studies and provided dramatic evidence that
an object could be matched to multiple visual memories
in parallel.
B) Memory Models and Findings:
Broadbent’s model of attention and memory stimulated
the formulation of rival models in the 1960s.
These models assumed that short-term memory (STM)
and long-term memory (LTM) were qualitatively
different structures, with information first entering
STM and then being transferred to LTM (e.g., Waugh
and Norman 1965). The Atkinson and Shiffrin (1968)
model proved particularly influential. With its emphases
on information flowing between memory
stores, control processes regulating that flow, and
mathematical descriptions of these processes, the
model was a quintessential example of the information-processing
approach. The model was related to
various findings about memory. For example, when
people have to recall a long list of words they do best
on the first words presented, a ‘primacy’ effect, and on
the last few words presented, a ‘recency’ effect. Various
experiments indicated that the recency effect reflected
retrieval from STM, whereas the primacy effect
reflected enhanced retrieval from LTM due to greater
rehearsal for the first items presented (e.g., Murdock
1962, Glanzer and Cunitz 1966). At the time these
results were seen as very supportive of dual-memory
models (although alternative interpretations would
soon be proposed—particularly by Craik and
Lockhart 1972). ly determining the characteristics of encoding,
storage, and retrieval processes in STM and LTM.
The results indicated that verbal material was encoded
and stored in a phonologic code for STM, but a more
meaning-based code for LTM (Conrad 1964, Kintsch
and Buschke 1969).
C) The New Psycho linguistics:
Beginning in the early 1960s there was great interest in
determining the psychological reality of Chomsky’s
theories of language (these theories had been formulated
with ideal listeners and speakers in mind). Some
of these linguistically inspired experiments presented
sentences in perception and memory paradigms, and
showed that sentences deemed more syntactically
complex by transnational grammar were harder to
perceive or store (Miller 1962). Subtler experiments
tried to show that syntactic units, like phrases,
functioned as units in perception, STM, and LTM
(Fodor et al. (1974) is the classic review). While many
of these results are no longer seen as critical, this
research effort created a new sub field of cognitive
psychology, a psycho linguistics that demanded sophistication
in modern linguistic theory.
Not all psycholinguistic studies focused on syntax.
Some dealt with semantics, particularly the representation
of the meanings of words, and a few of these
studies made use of the newly developed mental
chronometry. One experiment that proved seminal
was reported by Collins and Quillian (1969).
3. The Rise of Cognitive Science:
A) Memory and Language:
Early in the 1970s the fields of memory and language
began to intersect. In 1973 John Anderson and
Gordon Bower published Human Associative Memory
(Anderson and Bower 1973), which presented a model
of memory for linguistic materials. The model combined
information processing with recent developments
in linguistics and artificial intelligence (AI),
thereby linking the three major research directions
that led to the cognitive revolution. The model used
networks similar to that considered above to represent
semantic knowledge, and used memory-search processes
to interrogate these networks.
As psychologists became aware of related developments
in linguistics and artificial intelligence, so
researchers in the latter disciplines become aware of
pertinent work in psychology. Thus evolved the
interdisciplinary movement called ‘cognitive science.’
In addition to psychology, AI, and linguistics, the
fields of cultural anthropology and philosophy of
mind also became involved. The movement eventuated
in numerous interdisciplinary collaborations (e.g.,
Rumelhart et al. 1986), as well as in individual
psychologists becoming more interdisciplinary.
B) Representational Issues:
In the 1970s and early 1980s, cognitive science was
much concerned with issues about mental representations. Whereas the memory-for-language models described
earlier had assumed representations that were
language-like, or propositional, other researchers argued
that representations could also be imaginal, like
a visual image. Shepard and Cooper (1972) provided
evidence that people could mentally rotate their
representations of objects, and Kosslyn (1980) surveyed
numerous phenomena that further implicated
visual imagery. In keeping with the interdisciplinary
of cognitive science, AI researchers and philosophers
entered the debate about propositional versus
imaginal representations (e.g., Block 1981, Pylyshyn
1981). In addition to questions about the modality of
representations, there were concerns about the structure
of representations.
The cognitive science movement affected most areas
of cognitive psychology, ranging from object recognition
(Marr 1982) to reasoning (e.g., Johnson Laird
1983) to expertise in problem solving (e.g.,
Chase and Simon 1973). The movement continues to
be influential and increasingly focuses on computational
models of cognition. What has changed since its
inception in the 1970s is the kind of computational
model in favor.
4. Newer Directions: Connection-ism and Cognitive Neuroscience:
A) Connectionist Modeling:
The computer models that dominated cognitive psychology
from its inception used complex symbols as
representations, and processed these representations
in a rule-based fashion (for example, in a model of
object recognition, the representation for a frog might
consist of a conjunction of complex properties, and
the rule for recognition might look something like ‘If
it’s green, small, and croaks, it’s a frog’). Starting in
the early 1980s, an alternative kind of cognitive model
started to attract interest, namely ‘connectionist’ (or
‘parallel distributed processing’) models. These proposals
have the form of neural networks, consisting of
nodes (representations) that are densely interconnected,
with the connections varying in strength.
B) Cognitive Neuroscience:
The other major new direction in cognitive psychology
is the growing interest in the neural bases of cognition,
a movement referred to as ‘cognitive neuroscience.’
There had been little interest in biological work in the
research that brought about the cognitive revolution.
That early work was as much concerned with fighting
behaviorism as it was with advancing cognitive psychology,
and consequently much of the research focused
on higher-level processes and was completely
removed from anything going on in the neurobiology
of its day. Subsequent generations of cognitive psychologists
solidified their commitments to a purely
cognitive level of analyses, by arguing that the distinction
between cognitive and neural levels of analyses
was analogous to that between computer software and
hardware, and that cognitive psychology (and cognitive
science) was concerned primarily with the software.
Since the early 1990s, views about the importance
of neural analyses have changed dramatically.
There is a growing consensus that the standard
information processing analyses of cognition can be
substantially enlightened by knowing how cognition is
implemented in the brain.
5. Conclusion:
This article has given short shrift to important contributions
that tend to fall off the mainstream of cognitive
psychology. One such case is the work done by Dan
Kahneman and Amos Tversky (e.g., Kahneman and
Tversky 1973, Tversky and Kahneman 1983) on the
use of heuristics in decision making, which can result in deviations from rational behavior. Another example
is the cognitively inspired study of memory and
language deficits in neurological patients (Shallice
(1988) provides a review). There are other cases like
these which deserve a prominent place in a fuller
history of cognitive psychology.
Thanku
ReplyDeleteThis post is so usefull and informative.Keep updating with more information...
ReplyDeleteAI classes in Mumbai
Artificial Intelligence Institute in Ahmedabad
Artificial Intelligence Institute in Kochi
Artificial Intelligence Training in Trivandrum
AI Course in Kolkata